By GREGORY BERNS
Now, after training and scanning a dozen dogs, my one
inescapable conclusion is this: dogs are people, too.
Because dogs can’t speak, scientists have relied on
behavioral observations to infer what dogs are thinking. It is a tricky
business. You can’t ask a dog why he does something. And you certainly can’t ask
him how he feels. The prospect of ferreting out animal emotions scares many
scientists. After all, animal research is big business. It has been easy to
sidestep the difficult questions about animal sentience and emotions because
they have been unanswerable.
Until now.
By looking directly at their brains and bypassing the
constraints of behaviorism, M.R.I.’s can tell us about dogs’ internal states.
M.R.I.’s are conducted in loud, confined spaces. People don’t like them, and
you have to hold absolutely still during the procedure. Conventional veterinary
practice says you have to anesthetize animals so they don’t move during a scan.
But you can’t study brain function in an anesthetized animal. At least not
anything interesting like perception or emotion.
From the beginning, we treated the dogs as persons. We
had a consent form, which was modeled after a child’s consent form but signed
by the dog’s owner. We emphasized that participation was voluntary, and that
the dog had the right to quit the study. We used only positive training
methods. No sedation. No restraints. If the dogs didn’t want to be in the
M.R.I. scanner, they could leave. Same as any human volunteer.
My dog Callie was the first. Rescued from a shelter,
Callie was a skinny black terrier mix, what is called a feist in the southern
Appalachians, from where she came. True to her roots, she preferred hunting
squirrels and rabbits in the backyard to curling up in my lap. She had a
natural inquisitiveness, which probably landed her in the shelter in the first
place, but also made training a breeze.
With the help of my friend Mark Spivak, a dog trainer,
we started teaching Callie to go into an M.R.I. simulator that I built in my
living room. She learned to walk up steps into a tube, place her head in a
custom-fitted chin rest, and hold rock-still for periods of up to 30 seconds.
Oh, and she had to learn to wear earmuffs to protect her sensitive hearing from
the 95 decibels of noise the scanner makes.
After months of training and some trial-and-error at
the real M.R.I. scanner, we were rewarded with the first maps of brain
activity. For our first tests, we measured Callie’s brain response to two hand
signals in the scanner. In later experiments, not yet published, we determined
which parts of her brain distinguished the scents of familiar and unfamiliar
dogs and humans.
Soon, the local dog community learned of our quest to
determine what dogs are thinking. Within a year, we had assembled a team of a
dozen dogs who were all “M.R.I.-certified.”
Although we are just beginning to answer basic
questions about the canine brain, we cannot ignore the striking similarity
between dogs and humans in both the structure and function of a key brain
region: the caudate nucleus.
Rich in dopamine receptors, the caudate sits between
the brainstem and the cortex. In humans, the caudate plays a key role in the
anticipation of things we enjoy, like food, love and money. But can we flip
this association around and infer what a person is thinking just by measuring
caudate activity? Because of the overwhelming complexity of how different parts
of the brain are connected to one another, it is not usually possible to pin a
single cognitive function or emotion to a single brain region.
But the caudate may be an exception. Specific parts of
the caudate stand out for their consistent activation to many things that
humans enjoy. Caudate activation is so consistent that under the right
circumstances, it can predict our preferences for food, music and even beauty.
In dogs, we found that activity in the caudate
increased in response to hand signals indicating food. The caudate also
activated to the smells of familiar humans. And in preliminary tests, it
activated to the return of an owner who had momentarily stepped out of view. Do
these findings prove that dogs love us? Not quite. But many of the same things
that activate the human caudate, which are associated with positive emotions,
also activate the dog caudate. Neuroscientists call this a functional homology,
and it may be an indication of canine emotions.
The ability to experience positive emotions, like love
and attachment, would mean that dogs have a level of sentience comparable to
that of a human child. And this ability suggests a rethinking of how we treat
dogs.
DOGS have long been considered property. Though the
Animal Welfare Act of 1966 and state laws raised the bar for the treatment of
animals, they solidified the view that animals are things — objects that can be
disposed of as long as reasonable care is taken to minimize their suffering.
But now, by using the M.R.I. to push away the
limitations of behaviorism, we can no longer hide from the evidence. Dogs, and
probably many other animals (especially our closest primate relatives), seem to
have emotions just like us. And this means we must reconsider their treatment
as property.
One alternative is a sort of limited personhood for
animals that show neurobiological evidence of positive emotions. Many rescue
groups already use the label of “guardian” to describe human caregivers,
binding the human to his ward with an implicit responsibility to care for her.
Failure to act as a good guardian runs the risk of having the dog placed
elsewhere. But there are no laws that cover animals as wards, so the patchwork
of rescue groups that operate under a guardianship model have little legal foundation
to protect the animals’ interest.
If we went a step further and granted dogs rights of
personhood, they would be afforded additional protection against exploitation.
Puppy mills, laboratory dogs and dog racing would be banned for violating the
basic right of self-determination of a person.
I suspect that society is many years away from
considering dogs as persons. However, recent rulings by the Supreme Court have
included neuroscientific findings that open the door to such a possibility. In
two cases, the court ruled that juvenile offenders could not be sentenced to
life imprisonment without the possibility of parole. As part of the rulings,
the court cited brain-imaging evidence that the human brain was not mature in
adolescence. Although this case has nothing to do with dog sentience, the
justices opened the door for neuroscience in the courtroom.
Perhaps someday we may see a case arguing for a dog’s
rights based on brain-imaging findings.
Gregory Berns is a
professor of neuroeconomics at Emory University and the author of “How Dogs
Love Us: A Neuroscientist and His Adopted Dog Decode the Canine Brain.”
Nessun commento:
Posta un commento